| Peer-Reviewed

Self-reported Fear, Psychological Inflexibility and Opioid Dose in Relation to Pain

Received: 1 October 2021    Accepted: 20 October 2021    Published: 29 October 2021
Views:       Downloads:
Abstract

Within the past two decades, research on the treatment of chronic pain with opioid medication has attempted to understand the complex relationship between psychological factors, subjective pain experience, and prescription opioid use. Specifically, fear and psychological inflexibility factors have been explored in relation to both pain sensation and opioid dose. The current study aims to explore and enhance the understandings of brief, subjective self-report measures of fear in relation to opioid dose, subjective fear, self-reported pain, and psychological inflexibility processes. This study examined whether (1) a quadratic relationship would exist between pain scores and opioid dose; (2) individuals with higher opioid dosages would have higher psychological inflexibility scores and subjective fear of pain scores; (3) subjective fear scores, in concert with psychological inflexibility pain measures, would be predictive of pain scores, and (4) subjective fear scores would positively correlate to psychological inflexibility pain measures. The final sample consisted of 202 respondents of an online survey for chronic pain. Survey measures included the Chronic Pain Grade questionnaire (CPG), the Psychological Inflexibility in Pain Scale (PIPS), Subjective Fear of Pain when in Low Pain (FlowP), and when in No Pain (FnoP). Opioid dosage for each participant was converted to the standardized Morphine Milligram Equivalent (MME). A significant quadratic relationship between the CPG and MME was found (p=0.016). MME scores were ns in relation to Subjective Fear of Pain scores or PIPS. FlowP and FnoP, however, did predict overall pain scores for participants (p<0.001). Overall pain scores also showed a positive moderate relationship with overall PIPS scores (r(200)=0.673, p<0.001). FlowP and PIPS together explained 45.7% of the variance of pain scores (F(2,199) = 83.640, p=0.003, R=0.676, R2=0.457) with FnoP and PIPS explaining slightly less at 44.8% (F(2,187)=76.002, p<0.001, R=0.670, R2=0.448). FlowP, however, showed slightly stronger correlations to overall PIPS scores (r(200)=0.648, p<0.001) when compared to FnoP (r(188)=0.589, p<0.001). These findings support previous research indicating a quadratic relationship between pain and opioid dose. Higher pain scores were correlated to higher scores on PIPS and subjective fear of pain questions. Of benefit, the subjective fear of pain questions showed some minor predictability when used as a two-question predictor of pain. Our results not only support previous research underlying the relationship between opioid dose and pain but expand on insight into the use of short-form, fear-related questions to predict psychometrics such as psychological inflexibility and pain sensation.

Published in Psychology and Behavioral Sciences (Volume 10, Issue 5)
DOI 10.11648/j.pbs.20211005.13
Page(s) 178-182
Creative Commons

This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is properly cited.

Copyright

Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Science Publishing Group

Keywords

Psychological Flexibility, Pain, Opioid Use, Subjective Fear

References
[1] Vijay, A., Rhee, T. G., & Ross, J. S. (2019). US prescribing trends of fentanyl, opioids, and other pain medications in outpatient and emergency department visits from 2006 to 2015. Preventive Medicine: An International Journal Devoted to Practice and Theory, 123, 123–129. https://doi -org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2019.03.022
[2] Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2017, July 6). Opioid prescribing. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. https://www.cdc.gov/vitalsigns/opioids/infographic.html.
[3] Nicol, A. L., Hurley, R. W., & Benzon, H. T. (2017). Alternatives to Opioids in the Pharmacologic Management of Chronic Pain Syndromes: A Narrative Review of Randomized, Controlled, and Blinded Clinical Trials. Anesthesia and analgesia, 125 (5), 1682–1703.
[4] Rosenblum, A., Marsch, L. A., Joseph, H., & Portenoy, R. K. (2008). Opioids and the treatment of chronic pain: controversies, current status, and future directions. Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology, 16 (5), 405–416. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013628
[5] Ballantyne J. C. (2017). Opioids for the Treatment of Chronic Pain: Mistakes Made, Lessons Learned, and Future Directions. Anesthesia and analgesia, 125 (5), 1769–1778. https://doi.org/10.1213/ANE.0000000000002500
[6] Palermo, T. M., & Kerns, R. D. (2020). Psychology’s role in addressing the dual crises of chronic pain and opioid-related harms: Introduction to the special issue. American Psychologist, 75 (6), 741 747. https://doi-org.mwu.idm.oclc.org/10.1037/amp0000711
[7] Morasco, B. J., Yarborough, B. J., Smith, N. X., Dobscha, S. K., Deyo, R. A., Perrin, N. A., & Green, C. A. (2017). Higher prescription opioid dose is associated with worse patient-reported pain outcomes and more health care utilization. The Journal of Pain, 18 (4), 437-445. https://doi-org /10.1016/j.jpain.2016.12.004
[8] Buhrman, M., Skoglund, A., Husell, J., Bergström, K., Gordh, T., Hursti, T., Bendelin, N., Furmark, T., & Andersson, G. (2013). Guided internet-delivered acceptance and commitment therapy for chronic pain patients: A randomized controlled trial. Behavior Research and Therapy, 51 (6), 307-315. https://doi-org/10.1016/j. brat.2013.02.010
[9] Trompetter, H. R., Bohlmeijer, E. T., Veehof, M. M., & Schreurs, K. M. G. (2015). Internet-based guided self-help intervention for chronic pain based on Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 38 (1), 66-80. https://doi-org/10.1007/s10865-014-9579-0
[10] Hayes, S. C. (2002). Buddhism and acceptance and commitment therapy. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 9 (1), 58–66. https://doi-org /10.1016/S1077-7229(02)80041-4
[11] Fish, R. A., Hogan, M. J., Morrison, T. G., Stewart, I., & McGuire, B. E. (2013). Willing and able: A closer look at Pain Willingness and Activity Engagement on the Chronic Pain Acceptance Questionnaire (CPAQ-8). The Journal of Pain, 14 (3), 233–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2012.11.004
[12] Elsenbruch, S., Roderigo, T., Enck, P., & Benson, S. (2019). Can a brief relaxation exercise modulate placebo or nocebo effects in a visceral pain model? Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10. https://doi-org.mwu.idm.oclc.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00144
[13] Markfelder, T., & Pauli, P. (2020). Fear of pain and pain intensity: Meta-analysis and systematic review. Psychological Bulletin, 146 (5), 411–450. https://doi.org.mwu.idm.oclc.org/10.1037/bul0000228
[14] Von Korff, M., Ormel, J., Keefe, F. J., & Dworkin, S. F. (1992). Chronic Pain Grade Questionnaire [Database record]. Retrieved from PsycTESTS. doi: https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/t10391-000
[15] Wicksell, R. K., Renöfält, J., Olsson, G. L., Bond, F. W., & Melin, L. (2008). Psychological Inflexibility in Pain Scale. PsycTESTS. https://doi.org/10.1037/t23288-000
[16] Wicksell, R. K., Renöfält, J., Olsson, G. L., Bond, F. W., & Melin, L. (2008). Avoidance and cognitive fusion-Central components in pain related disability? Development and preliminary validation of the Psychological Inflexibility in Pain Scale (PIPS). European Journal of Pain, 12 (4), 491–500.
[17] Roderigo T, Benson S, Schöls M, Hetkamp M, Schedlowski M, Enck P, et al. (2017) Effects of acute psychological stress on placebo and nocebo responses in a clinically relevant model of visceroception. Pain. 158: 1489–98. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000940g/10.1016/j.ejpain.2007.08.003
[18] Asiri, F., Reddy, R. S., Tedla, J. S., ALMohiza, M. A., Alshahrani, M. S., Govindappa, S. C., & Sangadala, D. R. (2021). Kinesiophobia and its correlations with pain, proprioception, and functional performance among individuals with chronic neck pain. PLoS ONE, 16 (7). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0254262
[19] Varallo, G., Scarpina, F., Giusti, E. M., Cattivelli, R., Guerrini Usubini, A., Capodaglio, P., & Castelnuovo, G. (2021). Does Kinesiophobia Mediate the Relationship between Pain Intensity and Disability in Individuals with Chronic Low-Back Pain and Obesity? Brain Sciences, 11 (6). https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11060684
[20] McCracken, L. M., Patel, S., & Scott, W. (2018). The role of psychological flexibility in relation to suicidal thinking in chronic pain. European Journal of Pain, 22 (10), 1774–1781. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejp.1273
Cite This Article
  • APA Style

    Brandon Alton Scott, Thomas Barnes Virden III, Krista Perdue. (2021). Self-reported Fear, Psychological Inflexibility and Opioid Dose in Relation to Pain. Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, 10(5), 178-182. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20211005.13

    Copy | Download

    ACS Style

    Brandon Alton Scott; Thomas Barnes Virden III; Krista Perdue. Self-reported Fear, Psychological Inflexibility and Opioid Dose in Relation to Pain. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 2021, 10(5), 178-182. doi: 10.11648/j.pbs.20211005.13

    Copy | Download

    AMA Style

    Brandon Alton Scott, Thomas Barnes Virden III, Krista Perdue. Self-reported Fear, Psychological Inflexibility and Opioid Dose in Relation to Pain. Psychol Behav Sci. 2021;10(5):178-182. doi: 10.11648/j.pbs.20211005.13

    Copy | Download

  • @article{10.11648/j.pbs.20211005.13,
      author = {Brandon Alton Scott and Thomas Barnes Virden III and Krista Perdue},
      title = {Self-reported Fear, Psychological Inflexibility and Opioid Dose in Relation to Pain},
      journal = {Psychology and Behavioral Sciences},
      volume = {10},
      number = {5},
      pages = {178-182},
      doi = {10.11648/j.pbs.20211005.13},
      url = {https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20211005.13},
      eprint = {https://article.sciencepublishinggroup.com/pdf/10.11648.j.pbs.20211005.13},
      abstract = {Within the past two decades, research on the treatment of chronic pain with opioid medication has attempted to understand the complex relationship between psychological factors, subjective pain experience, and prescription opioid use. Specifically, fear and psychological inflexibility factors have been explored in relation to both pain sensation and opioid dose. The current study aims to explore and enhance the understandings of brief, subjective self-report measures of fear in relation to opioid dose, subjective fear, self-reported pain, and psychological inflexibility processes. This study examined whether (1) a quadratic relationship would exist between pain scores and opioid dose; (2) individuals with higher opioid dosages would have higher psychological inflexibility scores and subjective fear of pain scores; (3) subjective fear scores, in concert with psychological inflexibility pain measures, would be predictive of pain scores, and (4) subjective fear scores would positively correlate to psychological inflexibility pain measures. The final sample consisted of 202 respondents of an online survey for chronic pain. Survey measures included the Chronic Pain Grade questionnaire (CPG), the Psychological Inflexibility in Pain Scale (PIPS), Subjective Fear of Pain when in Low Pain (FlowP), and when in No Pain (FnoP). Opioid dosage for each participant was converted to the standardized Morphine Milligram Equivalent (MME). A significant quadratic relationship between the CPG and MME was found (p=0.016). MME scores were ns in relation to Subjective Fear of Pain scores or PIPS. FlowP and FnoP, however, did predict overall pain scores for participants (p2=0.457) with FnoP and PIPS explaining slightly less at 44.8% (F(2,187)=76.002, p2=0.448). FlowP, however, showed slightly stronger correlations to overall PIPS scores (r(200)=0.648, p<0.001) when compared to FnoP (r(188)=0.589, p<0.001). These findings support previous research indicating a quadratic relationship between pain and opioid dose. Higher pain scores were correlated to higher scores on PIPS and subjective fear of pain questions. Of benefit, the subjective fear of pain questions showed some minor predictability when used as a two-question predictor of pain. Our results not only support previous research underlying the relationship between opioid dose and pain but expand on insight into the use of short-form, fear-related questions to predict psychometrics such as psychological inflexibility and pain sensation.},
     year = {2021}
    }
    

    Copy | Download

  • TY  - JOUR
    T1  - Self-reported Fear, Psychological Inflexibility and Opioid Dose in Relation to Pain
    AU  - Brandon Alton Scott
    AU  - Thomas Barnes Virden III
    AU  - Krista Perdue
    Y1  - 2021/10/29
    PY  - 2021
    N1  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20211005.13
    DO  - 10.11648/j.pbs.20211005.13
    T2  - Psychology and Behavioral Sciences
    JF  - Psychology and Behavioral Sciences
    JO  - Psychology and Behavioral Sciences
    SP  - 178
    EP  - 182
    PB  - Science Publishing Group
    SN  - 2328-7845
    UR  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.pbs.20211005.13
    AB  - Within the past two decades, research on the treatment of chronic pain with opioid medication has attempted to understand the complex relationship between psychological factors, subjective pain experience, and prescription opioid use. Specifically, fear and psychological inflexibility factors have been explored in relation to both pain sensation and opioid dose. The current study aims to explore and enhance the understandings of brief, subjective self-report measures of fear in relation to opioid dose, subjective fear, self-reported pain, and psychological inflexibility processes. This study examined whether (1) a quadratic relationship would exist between pain scores and opioid dose; (2) individuals with higher opioid dosages would have higher psychological inflexibility scores and subjective fear of pain scores; (3) subjective fear scores, in concert with psychological inflexibility pain measures, would be predictive of pain scores, and (4) subjective fear scores would positively correlate to psychological inflexibility pain measures. The final sample consisted of 202 respondents of an online survey for chronic pain. Survey measures included the Chronic Pain Grade questionnaire (CPG), the Psychological Inflexibility in Pain Scale (PIPS), Subjective Fear of Pain when in Low Pain (FlowP), and when in No Pain (FnoP). Opioid dosage for each participant was converted to the standardized Morphine Milligram Equivalent (MME). A significant quadratic relationship between the CPG and MME was found (p=0.016). MME scores were ns in relation to Subjective Fear of Pain scores or PIPS. FlowP and FnoP, however, did predict overall pain scores for participants (p2=0.457) with FnoP and PIPS explaining slightly less at 44.8% (F(2,187)=76.002, p2=0.448). FlowP, however, showed slightly stronger correlations to overall PIPS scores (r(200)=0.648, p<0.001) when compared to FnoP (r(188)=0.589, p<0.001). These findings support previous research indicating a quadratic relationship between pain and opioid dose. Higher pain scores were correlated to higher scores on PIPS and subjective fear of pain questions. Of benefit, the subjective fear of pain questions showed some minor predictability when used as a two-question predictor of pain. Our results not only support previous research underlying the relationship between opioid dose and pain but expand on insight into the use of short-form, fear-related questions to predict psychometrics such as psychological inflexibility and pain sensation.
    VL  - 10
    IS  - 5
    ER  - 

    Copy | Download

Author Information
  • Clinical Psychology, Midwestern University, Glendale, USA

  • Clinical Psychology, Midwestern University, Glendale, USA

  • Clinical Psychology, Midwestern University, Glendale, USA

  • Sections