American Journal of Health Research

| Peer-Reviewed |

Reaction of Men to Breast-Implants of Women

Received: 2 October 2016    Accepted: 19 October 2016    Published: 9 November 2016
Views:       Downloads:

Share This Article

Abstract

Goal of this study was the investigation of men’s attitudes toward breast-implants of women. It was examined which opinion men have in regard to artificial filled breasts and whether a preference for big breasts exists, even if the breasts are filled with an implant. A questionnaire was developed, which registered men’s opinions concerning breast-implants; it included 12 items on a Likert Scale; mostly the items were bi- or unipolar, others were asked as open text. The inner consistency of the items was Cronbach's Alpha = 0.741. For the investigation of personality factors the Freiburger Personality Inventory (FPI-R) was used. These questionnaires were published via SosciSurvey, and spread through distribution in social networks. The original sample of n=109 included exclusively male persons between 18-54 years. After use of exclusion criteria, ultimately 81 sets of data were evaluable. Results showed that most men have a preference for big breasts (p<0.01); but this was not valid for artificial enlarged breasts - here many participants had a neutral attitude and the difference between the number of supporters and opponents was not significant (p-values between 0.11 and 0.92). The status of the relationship and the duration of being a single had no relation to the attitudes toward female breast-implants. The hypothesis that men without a partner are more willing to accept breast-implants than men in a relationship rendered only p-values between 0.56 and 0.96 (n.s.). Even the hypothesis "The longer the duration of being single, the higher the acceptance of breast-implants" showed only non-significant p-values between 0.28 and 0.84. Moreover, the classification of the own attractiveness (p=0.297, n.s.) as well as the score on the extraversion-introversion scale did not affect the attitude toward breast-implants (p=0.346, n.s.). The here presented data show that about half of men nowadays accept artificial enlarged breasts in women. Most distributions had multiple summits, i.e. the participants could be divided in (a) supporters, (b) opponents and (c) a neither/nor group to whom the girl behind the breast is more important. Whether a man is able to accept breast-implants in his girl-friend seems to be a very stable attitude, which is hardly influenced by personality dimensions. Even the duration of a life without a female partner has no significant impact on this attitude.

DOI 10.11648/j.ajhr.20160406.14
Published in American Journal of Health Research (Volume 4, Issue 6, November 2016)
Page(s) 172-178
Creative Commons

This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is properly cited.

Copyright

Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Science Publishing Group

Keywords

Mamma Augmentation, Breast Enlargement, Breast Implants

References
[1] Alderman, A., Pusic, A. & Murphy, D. K. (2016): Prospective Analysis of Primary Breast Augmentation on Body Image Using the BREAST-Q: Results from a Nationwide Study. Plast Reconstr Surg. 137 (6): 954e-60e. doi: 10.1097/PRS.0000000000002183.
[2] Badura H. O. (1983): Zum somatopsychischen Einfluß plastischer Brustoperationen. [Psychosomatic effect of mammoplasty]. In: Psychotherapie, Psychosomatik, Medizinische Psychologie. 1983, 33 (5): 179-182.
[3] Bruck J. C. Kleinschmidt, A. & Ottomann, C. (2011): Gesteigertes Selbstvertrauen und vermindertes körperlich-sexuelles Missempfinden nach subpektoraler Breast augmentation [Increased Self-Confidence and Decreased Sexual Discomfort after Subpectoral Mammaplasty]. J Handchir Mikrochir plast Chir. 43 (2): 112-118. DOI: 10.1055/s-0030-1265164.
[4] Crerand, C. E. & Magee, L. (2013): Cosmetic and reconstructive breast surgery in adolescents: psychological, ethical, and legal considerations. Semin Plast Surg. 27 (1): 72-78. doi: 10.1055/s-0033-1343999.
[5] Dini, G. M., Gozzano, R., Ferreira, R. B., Faria, M., Farinazzo, M. & Ferreira, L. M. (2014): Mirror, mirror on the wall, which breast is bigger of them all? Plast Reconstr Surg Glob Open. 6; 2 (2): e105. doi: 10.1097/GOX.0000000000000037.
[6] Fahrenberg, J., Hampel, R. & Selg, H. (1984): Freiburger Persönlichkeitsinventar. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
[7] Figueroa-Haas, C. (2009): Psychological issues associated with breast augmentation. Issues Ment Health Nurs. 30 (6): 377-82. doi: 10.1080/01612840802488657.
[8] Guimaraes, P. A., Resende, V. C., Sabino Neto, M., Seito, C. L., de Brito, M. J., Abla, L. E., Veiga, D. F. & Ferreira, L. M. (2015): Sexuality in aesthetic breast surgery. Aesthetic Plast Surg. 39 (6): 993-9. doi: 10.1007/s00266-015-0574-9.
[9] Horch, R. E. & Kneser, U. (2012): Plastische Chirurgie. In. Siewert & Stein (Eds.): Chirurgie. Heidelberg: Springer.
[10] Jordan, S. W. & Corcoran, J. (2013): Considerations in breast augmentation in the adolescent patient. Semin Plast Surg. 27 (1): 67-71. doi: 10.1055/s-0033-1343998.
[11] Kalaaji, A., Bjertness, C. B., Nordahl, C. & Olafsen, K. (2013): Survey of breast implant patients: characteristics, depression rate, and quality of life. Aesthet Surg J. 33 (2): 252-7. doi: 10.1177/1090820X12473106.
[12] Larson, K. & Gosain, A. K. (2012): Cosmetic surgery in the adolescent patient. Plast Reconstr Surg. 129 (1): 135e-141e. doi: 10.1097/PRS.0b013e3182362bb8.
[13] Morris, D. (2004): Die nackte Eva [The naked Eva]. München: Wilhelm Heyne Verlag.
[14] Nikolic, J., Janjic, Z., Marinkovic, M. Petrovic, J. & Bozic, T. (2013): Psychosocial characteristics and motivational factors in woman seeking cosmetic breast augmentation surgery. Vojnosanit Pregl. 70 (10): 940-946.
[15] Reichenberger, M. A., Biedermann, N. & Germann, G. (2011): Ästhetische Brustvergrößerung [Aesthetic breast augmentation]. Der Chirurg. 82: 782. DOI: 10.1007/s00104-011-2108-6.
[16] Reinboth, C. (2008): SampleSizer: Tool zur Bestimmung des optimalen Stichprobenumfangs. In: Statistikberatung. URL: http://statistikberatung.blogspot.de/2008/03/samplesizer-kostenloses-tool-zur.html (abgerufen am 13. August 2014).
[17] Palma, A. F., Zuk, G., Raptis, D. A., Franck, S., Eylert, G., Frueh, F. S., Guggenheim, M. & Shafighi, M. (2016): Quality of information for women seeking breast augmentation in the Internet. J Plast Surg Hand Surg. 50 (5): 262-71. doi: 10.3109/2000656X.2016.1154469.
[18] Saariniemi, K. M., Helle, H. M., Salmi, A. M., Peltoniemi, H. H., Charpentier, P. & Kuokkanen, H. O. (2012): The effects of aesthetic breast augmentation on quality of life, psychological distress, and eating disorder symptoms: a prospective study. Aesthetic Plast Surg. 36 (5): 1090-5. doi: 10.1007/s00266-012-9917-y.
[19] Saiga, M., Taira, N., Kimata, Y., Watanabe, S., Mukai, Y., Shimozuma, K., Mizoo, T., Nogami, T., Iwamoto, T., Motoki, T., Shien, T. Matsuoka, J. & Doihara, H. (2016): Development of a Japanese version of the BREAST-Q and the traditional psychometric test of the mastectomy module for the assessment of HRQOL and patient satisfaction following breast surgery. Breast Cancer. DOI 10.1007/s12282-016-0703-6.
[20] Schipperges; I. & und Simon, V. (2014): Bin ich nicht schön? Schönheitsideale der Kulturen. [Am I beauty? Ideals of attractiveness of cultures]. In: Süddeutsche Zeitung. Stand 2010. URL: http://www.sueddeutsche.de/leben/schoenheitsideale-der-kulturen-bin-ich-nicht-schoen-1.204145 (10. Juni 2014).
Cite This Article
  • APA Style

    Katharina Grundmann, Klaus Hoffmann, Erich Kasten. (2016). Reaction of Men to Breast-Implants of Women. American Journal of Health Research, 4(6), 172-178. https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajhr.20160406.14

    Copy | Download

    ACS Style

    Katharina Grundmann; Klaus Hoffmann; Erich Kasten. Reaction of Men to Breast-Implants of Women. Am. J. Health Res. 2016, 4(6), 172-178. doi: 10.11648/j.ajhr.20160406.14

    Copy | Download

    AMA Style

    Katharina Grundmann, Klaus Hoffmann, Erich Kasten. Reaction of Men to Breast-Implants of Women. Am J Health Res. 2016;4(6):172-178. doi: 10.11648/j.ajhr.20160406.14

    Copy | Download

  • @article{10.11648/j.ajhr.20160406.14,
      author = {Katharina Grundmann and Klaus Hoffmann and Erich Kasten},
      title = {Reaction of Men to Breast-Implants of Women},
      journal = {American Journal of Health Research},
      volume = {4},
      number = {6},
      pages = {172-178},
      doi = {10.11648/j.ajhr.20160406.14},
      url = {https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajhr.20160406.14},
      eprint = {https://article.sciencepublishinggroup.com/pdf/10.11648.j.ajhr.20160406.14},
      abstract = {Goal of this study was the investigation of men’s attitudes toward breast-implants of women. It was examined which opinion men have in regard to artificial filled breasts and whether a preference for big breasts exists, even if the breasts are filled with an implant. A questionnaire was developed, which registered men’s opinions concerning breast-implants; it included 12 items on a Likert Scale; mostly the items were bi- or unipolar, others were asked as open text. The inner consistency of the items was Cronbach's Alpha = 0.741. For the investigation of personality factors the Freiburger Personality Inventory (FPI-R) was used. These questionnaires were published via SosciSurvey, and spread through distribution in social networks. The original sample of n=109 included exclusively male persons between 18-54 years. After use of exclusion criteria, ultimately 81 sets of data were evaluable. Results showed that most men have a preference for big breasts (p<0.01); but this was not valid for artificial enlarged breasts - here many participants had a neutral attitude and the difference between the number of supporters and opponents was not significant (p-values between 0.11 and 0.92). The status of the relationship and the duration of being a single had no relation to the attitudes toward female breast-implants. The hypothesis that men without a partner are more willing to accept breast-implants than men in a relationship rendered only p-values between 0.56 and 0.96 (n.s.). Even the hypothesis "The longer the duration of being single, the higher the acceptance of breast-implants" showed only non-significant p-values between 0.28 and 0.84. Moreover, the classification of the own attractiveness (p=0.297, n.s.) as well as the score on the extraversion-introversion scale did not affect the attitude toward breast-implants (p=0.346, n.s.). The here presented data show that about half of men nowadays accept artificial enlarged breasts in women. Most distributions had multiple summits, i.e. the participants could be divided in (a) supporters, (b) opponents and (c) a neither/nor group to whom the girl behind the breast is more important. Whether a man is able to accept breast-implants in his girl-friend seems to be a very stable attitude, which is hardly influenced by personality dimensions. Even the duration of a life without a female partner has no significant impact on this attitude.},
     year = {2016}
    }
    

    Copy | Download

  • TY  - JOUR
    T1  - Reaction of Men to Breast-Implants of Women
    AU  - Katharina Grundmann
    AU  - Klaus Hoffmann
    AU  - Erich Kasten
    Y1  - 2016/11/09
    PY  - 2016
    N1  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajhr.20160406.14
    DO  - 10.11648/j.ajhr.20160406.14
    T2  - American Journal of Health Research
    JF  - American Journal of Health Research
    JO  - American Journal of Health Research
    SP  - 172
    EP  - 178
    PB  - Science Publishing Group
    SN  - 2330-8796
    UR  - https://doi.org/10.11648/j.ajhr.20160406.14
    AB  - Goal of this study was the investigation of men’s attitudes toward breast-implants of women. It was examined which opinion men have in regard to artificial filled breasts and whether a preference for big breasts exists, even if the breasts are filled with an implant. A questionnaire was developed, which registered men’s opinions concerning breast-implants; it included 12 items on a Likert Scale; mostly the items were bi- or unipolar, others were asked as open text. The inner consistency of the items was Cronbach's Alpha = 0.741. For the investigation of personality factors the Freiburger Personality Inventory (FPI-R) was used. These questionnaires were published via SosciSurvey, and spread through distribution in social networks. The original sample of n=109 included exclusively male persons between 18-54 years. After use of exclusion criteria, ultimately 81 sets of data were evaluable. Results showed that most men have a preference for big breasts (p<0.01); but this was not valid for artificial enlarged breasts - here many participants had a neutral attitude and the difference between the number of supporters and opponents was not significant (p-values between 0.11 and 0.92). The status of the relationship and the duration of being a single had no relation to the attitudes toward female breast-implants. The hypothesis that men without a partner are more willing to accept breast-implants than men in a relationship rendered only p-values between 0.56 and 0.96 (n.s.). Even the hypothesis "The longer the duration of being single, the higher the acceptance of breast-implants" showed only non-significant p-values between 0.28 and 0.84. Moreover, the classification of the own attractiveness (p=0.297, n.s.) as well as the score on the extraversion-introversion scale did not affect the attitude toward breast-implants (p=0.346, n.s.). The here presented data show that about half of men nowadays accept artificial enlarged breasts in women. Most distributions had multiple summits, i.e. the participants could be divided in (a) supporters, (b) opponents and (c) a neither/nor group to whom the girl behind the breast is more important. Whether a man is able to accept breast-implants in his girl-friend seems to be a very stable attitude, which is hardly influenced by personality dimensions. Even the duration of a life without a female partner has no significant impact on this attitude.
    VL  - 4
    IS  - 6
    ER  - 

    Copy | Download

Author Information
  • Medical School Hamburg, University of Applied Science & Medical University, Hamburg, Germany

  • Klinik für Dermatologie und Allergologie der Ruhr Universit?t, St.-Josef-Hospital, Bochum, Germany

  • Medical School Hamburg, University of Applied Science & Medical University, Hamburg, Germany

  • Sections